Which of the following refers to the conscious memory of facts and experiences?

  1. Memory
  2. Long-Term Memory
  3. Declarative Memory

By Ayesh Perera, published January 26, 2021


Take-home Messages

  • Long-term memory is not a single store and has two components: declarative (explicit) and non-declarative (implicit). Implicit memory (non-declarative) includes procedural memory and things learned through conditioning.
  • Declarative memory has to do with the storage of facts, and events we have personally experienced. Recalling information from declarative memory involves some degree of conscious effort, as information must be consciously brought to mind and “declared”.
  • Episodic memory and semantic memory are components of long-term memory known as explicit or declarative memory. Semantic memory involves the recall of ideas, concepts, and facts commonly regarded as general knowledge. Episodic memory, on the other hand, involves the recollection of personal events or episodes in a person's life, such as birthdays.
  • Throughout our lifetime and there is a gradual transition from episodic to semantic memories.

Which of the following refers to the conscious memory of facts and experiences?

Declarative memory is part of long-term memory involving “knowing that”, for example, London is the capital of England, zebras are animals, and the date of your mum's birthday (Cohen and Squire, 1980).

Declarative memory is also known as explicit memory, as it consists of information that is explicitly stored and involves conscious effort to be retrieved. This means that you are consciously aware when you are storing and recalling information.

Types of Declarative Memory

Episodic memory together with semantic memory is part of the division of memory known as explicit or declarative memory.

While episodic memory involves a person’s autobiographical experiences and associated events, semantic memory involves facts, concepts and skills acquired over time.

Episodic Memory

Episodic memory is part of long-term declarative memory, and comprises a person’s unique recollection of experiences, events, and situations.

The Canadian psychologist Endel Tulving first introduced the term ‘episodic memory’, in 1972, to distinguish ‘remembering’ from ‘knowing.’

Specific events, general events, personal facts, and flashbulb memories constitute different types of episodic memory.

They are a person’s unique memory of a specific event so it will be different from someone else’s recollection of the same experience, e.g., your first day of school.

Episodic memory has 3 elements: specific details of the event (time and place), context (what happened next), and emotions (how you felt).

Examples of episodic memory include: recalling your first abroad; remembering where you were when you heard that Mr. Trump had won the 2016 election; and the memory of your first day in college.


Types of Episodic Memory:
  • Specific events involve the recollection of particular moments from an individual’s autobiographical history. Recalling the first time you dove into the ocean is an example.
  • General events involve recalling the feelings associated with a certain type of experience. Recalling what it is like to dive into the ocean in general is an example of this type of episodic memory. You may not remember each occasion wherein you dove into the ocean. But you do have a general recollection of having dived many times into ocean—upon which your feeling is based.
  • Information intricately tied to a person’s experiences constitute personal facts. Knowing the color of your first bicycle and the name of your first dog are some examples.
  • Flashbulb memories are exceptionally vivid and highly detailed ‘snapshots’ of moments or circumstances wherein you learned important or surprising pieces of news (Brown & Kulik, 1977). Recalling the moment you heard about the death of a family member or a major tragedy such as the 9/11 attacks might be an example. It should be noted that there is much debate as to whether the vividness of a flashbulb memory stems from a virtual flash produced by the emotional intensity of a specific experience, or from a propensity to rehearse consequential moments—which can immensely strengthen the memory.

Neural Networks

It is possible to store episodic memories in auto-associative neural networks provided that the representation of the stored memories contains information about the spatiotemporal context wherein the representation was examined (Khalil, Moftah & Moustafa, 2017).

Neural networks which enhance the comprehension of the transmission and the reception of various messages to and from the body, comprise interconnected structures or neurons which harmoniously produce different intra-brain cognitions (Henderson, 2012).

Additionally, these networks can contract or expand based on the type of information being processed at a given time (Nestor, Kubicki, Gurrera, Niznikiewicz, Frumin, McCarley & Shenton, 2004).

Semantic Memory

Semantic memory a type of long-term declarative memory, and comprises facts about the world that are not linked to particular events or contexts. Semantic memory involves "knowing that" (e.g., Paris is the capital of France).

The Canadian psychologist Endel Tulving first introduced the term ‘semantic memory’, in 1972, to distinguish ‘remembering’ from ‘knowing.’

Semantic memory concerns knowledge about the world which is shared by everyone rather than personal knowledge, this may relate to the functions of objects, what behavior is appropriate, or even abstract concepts such as maths.

Some examples of semantic memory:
  • Recalling that Washington, D.C., is the U.S. capital and Washington is a state.
  • Recalling that April 1564 is the date on which Shakespeare was born.
  • Recalling the type of food people in ancient Egypt used to eat.
  • Knowing that elephants and giraffes are both mammals.

Episodic and Semantic Memory

Together, episodic memory and semantic memory constitute explicit or declarative memory, which is part of long-term memory. Episodic memory involves a person’s recollection of temporally dated information that permits the agent to mentally travel back in time and associate emotions with experiences.

Semantic memory, on the other hand, involves a structure of recorded skills, facts and concepts acquired overtime—via the accumulation of episodic memories. For instance, while each visit to Paris may constitute an episodic memory, the experiences of how Paris looks would constitute the semantic representation of the word ‘Paris.’

Furthermore, contrary to Tulving’s original proposal that episodic memory and semantic memory competed against each other for retrieval, more recent research based on experiments on LSA (Latent Semantic Analysis) suggests that the strength of semantic cues on retrieval are positively correlated with the strength of episodic cues (Howard & Kahana, 2002).

Additionally, impacts on episodic memory seem to affect semantic memory. For instance, damage wrought to the medial temporal lobe through conditions such as anterograde amnesia, impairs both episodic memory and semantic memory (Tulving & Markowitsch, 1998).


About the Author

Ayesh Perera recently graduated from Harvard University, where he studied politics, ethics and religion. He is presently conducting research in neuroscience and peak performance as an intern for the Cambridge Center for Behavioral Studies, while also working on a book of his own on constitutional law and legal interpretation.

How to reference this article:

Prera, A (2021, Jan 26). Declarative memory. Simply Psychology. www.simplypsychology.org/declarative-memory.html

APA Style References

Berryhill, M. E., Phuong, L., Picasso, L., Cabeza, R., & Olson, I. R. (2007). Parietal lobe and episodic memory: bilateral damage causes impaired free recall of autobiographical memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(52), 14415-14423.

Brown, R., & Kulik, J. (1977). Flashbulb memories. Cognition, 5(1), 73-99.

Clayton, N. S., Salwiczek, L. H., & Dickinson, A. (2007). Episodic memory. Current Biology, 17(6), R189-R191.

Cohen, N. J., & Squire, L. R. (1980). Preserved learning and retention of pattern analyzing skill in amnesia: Dissociation of knowing how and knowing that. Science, 210, 207–209.

Deisseroth, K., Singla, S., Toda, H., Monje, M., Palmer, T. D., & Malenka, R. C. (2004). Excitation-neurogenesis coupling in adult neural stem/progenitor cells. Neuron, 42(4), 535-552.

Grainger, A. (2008). Difficulties in tracking the long-term global trend in tropical forest area. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(2), 818-823.

Henderson, J. M. (2012). “Connectomic surgery”: diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) tractography as a targeting modality for surgical modulation of neural networks. Frontiers in integrative neuroscience, 6, 15.

Howard, M. W., & Kahana, M. J. (2002). When does semantic similarity help episodic retrieval?. Journal of Memory and Language, 46(1), 85-98.

Janowsky, J. S., Shimamura, A. P., & Squire, L. R. (1989). Source memory impairment in patients with frontal lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia, 27(8), 1043-1056.

Khalil, R., Moftah, M. Z., & Moustafa, A. A. (2017). The effects of dynamical synapses on firing rate activity: a spiking neural network model. European Journal of Neuroscience, 46(9), 2445-2470.

Maguire, E. A., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Aging affects the engagement of the hippocampus during autobiographical memory retrieval. Brain, 126(7), 1511-1523.

McCloskey, M., Wible, C. G., & Cohen, N. J. (1988). Is there a special flashbulb-memory mechanism?. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117(2), 171.

Nestor, P. G., Kubicki, M., Gurrera, R. J., Niznikiewicz, M., Frumin, M., McCarley, R. W., & Shenton, M. E. (2004). Neuropsychological correlates of diffusion tensor imaging in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology, 18(4), 629.

Scarf, D., Gross, J., Colombo, M., & Hayne, H. (2013). To have and to hold: Episodic memory in 3‐and 4‐year‐old children. Developmental psychobiology, 55(2), 125-132.

Schacter, D. L., Gilbert, D. T., & Wegner, D. M. (2011). Semantic and episodic memory. Psychology, 2, 240-1.

Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tulving & W. Donaldson (Eds.), Organization of Memory, (pp. 381–403). New York: Academic Press.

Tulving, E. (1985). Memory and consciousness. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie canadienne, 26(1), 1.

Tulving, E. (2002). Chronesthesia: Conscious Awareness of Subjective Time. Principles of Frontal Lobe Function, pp. 311–325.

Tulving, E. (2010). Précis of Elements of episodic memory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7(2), 223–238.

Tulving, E., & Markowitsch, H. J. (1998). Episodic and declarative memory: role of the hippocampus. Hippocampus, 8(3), 198-204.

Further Information

Semantic Memory Episodic Memory Clayton, N. S., Salwiczek, L. H., & Dickinson, A. (2007). Episodic memory. Current Biology, 17(6), R189-R191. Cohen, N. J., & Squire, L. R. (1980). Preserved learning and retention of pattern analyzing skill in amnesia: Dissociation of knowing how and knowing that. Science, 210, 207–209. Long-Term Memory Procedural Memory Implicit vs Explicit Memory

How to reference this article:

Prera, A (2021, Jan 26). Declarative memory. Simply Psychology. www.simplypsychology.org/declarative-memory.html

Home | About Us | Privacy Policy | Advertise | Contact Us

Simply Psychology's content is for informational and educational purposes only. Our website is not intended to be a substitute for professional medical advice, diagnosis, or treatment.

© Simply Scholar Ltd - All rights reserved

Which of the following refers to the conscious memory of facts and experiences?

What type of memory refers to the conscious memory of facts and experiences?

Explicit memory (or declarative memory) is one of the two main types of long-term human memory, the other of which is implicit memory. Explicit memory is the conscious, intentional recollection of factual information, previous experiences, and concepts.

Which of the following refers to the conscious memory of facts and experiences quizlet?

We are consciously aware of declarative memories. Memory of facts and experiences that one can consciously know and "declare", Information or knowledge that can be consciously recollected; also called declarative memory.

What term refers to the conscious recollection of facts or experiences that have been encountered in the past?

Episodic memory refers to a neurocognitive system that renders possible the conscious recollection of events as they were previously experienced. The term is closely related, but not identical, to autobiographical memory, which refers to memory for and about a person's own life.

What is an example of a conscious memory?

When do you use explicit memory? You use explicit memory when you are consciously recalling information. Examples include recalling your memories of events from your life, remembering information you have learned when taking a test, and recollecting upcoming appointments.