Some studies suggest that social support as a buffer for stress related disease is:

Open Access

Peer-reviewed

Research Article

  • Naomi I. Eisenberger

Unpacking the buffering effect of social support figures: Social support attenuates fear acquisition

  • Erica A. Hornstein, 
  • Naomi I. Eisenberger

Some studies suggest that social support as a buffer for stress related disease is:

x

  • Published: May 2, 2017
  • https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0175891

Figures

Abstract

Social support is associated with positive health outcomes, and research has demonstrated that the presence, or even just a reminder, of a social-support figure can reduce psychological and physiological responses to threats. However, the mechanisms underlying this effect are unclear, and no previous work has examined the impact of social support on basic fear learning processes, which have implications for threat responding. This study examined whether social support inhibits the formation of fear associations. After conducting a fear-conditioning procedure in which social-support stimuli were paired with conditional stimuli during fear acquisition, we found that the threat of shock was not associated with conditional stimuli paired with images of social-support figures, but was associated with stimuli paired with images of strangers. These findings indicate that social support prevents the formation of fear associations, reducing the amount of learned fears people acquire as they navigate the world, consequently reducing threat-related stress.

Citation: Hornstein EA, Eisenberger NI (2017) Unpacking the buffering effect of social support figures: Social support attenuates fear acquisition. PLoS ONE 12(5): e0175891. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0175891

Editor: Kirk Warren Brown, Virginia Commonwealth University, UNITED STATES

Received: August 30, 2016; Accepted: April 2, 2017; Published: May 2, 2017

Copyright: © 2017 Hornstein, Eisenberger. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

Data Availability: The de-identified data can be accessed at the Harvard Dataverse with DOI 10.7910/DVN/3WYVHE and at https://dataverse.harvard.edu/dataset.xhtml?persistentId=doi:10.7910/DVN/3WYVHE.

Funding: This work was supported by National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship (DGE-0707424) (EAH); Wendell Jeffrey and Bernice Wenzel Term Chair in Behavioral Neuroscience, University of California, Los Angeles (NIE); National Science Foundation (#1626477) (NIE). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Introduction

Research has consistently demonstrated a relationship between social support and positive health outcomes. It has been suggested that these health advantages arise, in part, because social support provides a buffer for individuals when dealing with life stress, and findings have shown that social support buffers against both the psychological and physiological threat response. Indeed, within the social buffering literature, it has been shown that individuals who have larger social networks, higher quality social relationships, and more access to social support resources have better physical and mental health, enjoying advantages ranging from a lower susceptibility to the common cold to a decreased risk of disease and death [1–3]. However, while this literature has established the impact of social support as a buffer, little prior work has examined the mechanisms whereby social support reduces physiological or psychological responses to threat. Consequently, the process by which social support provides this buffer remains not well understood. The present research seeks to explore this relationship by testing whether social support inhibits the formation of fear associations, consequently reducing fear responding and threat-related stress.

Evidence for this stress-buffering hypothesis can be found in both the animal and human literatures, and findings demonstrate that social support reduces both the psychological and physiological impact of threats. Animal research has shown that that the presence of familiar or close others decreases both the amount of escape and avoidance behavior exhibited in threatening contexts [4,5] decreases the amount of freezing behavior in response to a known threat [6], increases the ability to tolerate new environments [7,8], and decreases the amount of anxious behaviors exhibited following an experience of social defeat [9–11]. In addition to reducing behavioral and emotional stress responses, the presence of a familiar other can ameliorate physiological stress responses in the face of threatening events or situations. For example, the presence of a member of the same species with whom there is a bond reduces levels of cortisol when guinea pigs experience novel environments [12,13].

Consistent with the animal research on social buffering, work with humans has demonstrated that social support provides a similar buffering effect in threatening or stressful contexts. Findings show that perceptions of strong social support systems or relationships lead to reduced psychological stress in response to negative events [14–16]. Moreover, having higher levels of reported daily social support is correlated with reduced cortisol levels when faced with social stressors [17] as well as reduced heart rate and blood pressure in the face of acute stressors [18–20]. Additionally, it has been demonstrated that social support can provide a buffer for individuals by mitigating the experience of pain [21–23]. Recent work suggests that this pain-mitigating effect may be due to decreased activity in neural regions associated with the distressing aspect of pain and increased activity in neural regions associated with safety [24]. Altogether, these findings point to the important role played by social support in regulating stress in the face of threat, leading to lower behavioral and physiological reactivity, and possibly resulting in fewer negative downstream health consequences.

One possible mechanism by which social support provides this buffer against stress is by acting as a powerful natural safety signal—communicating protection and consequently reducing psychological and physiological threat responses. Indeed, recent research has shown that social-support figures are one category of prepared safety stimuli, less easily becoming associated with threat and reducing conditional fear responses, and that the presence of social-support figure reminders potentially leads to longer lasting fear extinction [25]. Thus, by signaling safety and interfering with normal fear learning processes, social support may reduce threat-related stress and increase positive health outcomes.

However, to date, no work has examined the effect of social support on the way fear is learned for other events or stimuli in the environment. It is possible that social support not only signals safety and reduces fear responding, but also decreases the amount of fear associations formed overall. Therefore, we designed a study to examine the impact of social support on fear learning, examining the effect of social-support-figure stimuli on the association of threat with other cues and testing whether social-support stimuli buffer individuals against acquiring new fears.

In order to test the impact of social support on fear learning, we used a fear-conditioning paradigm to examine whether the presence of social-support figure stimuli, defined here as the individual from whom a participant receives the most social support (in the form of care and resources) on a daily basis, reduced fear acquisition for a separate neutral cue. Specifically, we assessed conditional fear responses when a social-support figure’s image, or a stranger’s image, was paired with a neutral cue during fear acquisition. We hypothesized that while a conditional fear response would be acquired for neutral stimuli paired with images of strangers, no conditional fear response would be acquired for neutral stimuli paired with images of social-support figures.

Methods

Participants

Data were analyzed from a final sample of 20 participants (mean age = 19.70, 15 females) who completed the study procedures. This sample size was chosen based on a priori power analyses (see supplemental materials). In total, 30 participants were recruited, 2 participants were excluded based on the telephone screening, 4 were excluded based on the SCR screening, and 4 were excluded due equipment malfunction. All participants were recruited from the UCLA community and provided written consent. All consent and experimental procedures were approved by the UCLA IRB (#11–000896).

Procedure

The study had three parts: telephone screening, pre-screening session in the lab, and experimental session. Participants first completed the telephone screening session and pre-screening session to determine if they were eligible to participate in the experimental session (see supplemental materials). During the pre-screening session, they were asked to select “the individual who gives you the most support on a daily basis” and were instructed that these individuals could come from any relationship (e.g. parent, friend, significant other). They then were asked to rate how much social support this individual gives everyday on a scale of 1–10 (mean rating = 8.60). They were then instructed to send a digital photograph of this individual to the experimenter before the experimental session.

For the experimental session, participants returned to the lab and first completed a shock calibration procedure in order to determine the level of shock to be used for each individual participant during the experiment, such that it was extremely uncomfortable, but not painful (see supplemental materials). Participants then underwent a fear-conditioning session with 2 sets of stimuli. Each set comprised 2 neutral images from one of two object categories (clocks, stools), with one image from each set becoming a CS+ and one becoming a CS-, and both being paired with the same secondary image (social-support figure, stranger) during the acquisition stage of the experiment. There were three stages of the experiment: Habituation, Acquisition, and Extinction. For each stage, images were presented for 6 s, followed by a 10-s inter-stimulus-interval in a pseudo-random presentation order that was counter-balanced across participants. Fear responses were evaluated using Skin Conductance Response (SCR) measurements.

During the Habituation stage of the experiment, participants saw 3 non-reinforced presentations of each neutral image. This was done in order to ensure that there were no pre-existing characteristics of either of the neutral stimuli in each set that might account for later differences in SCR, and none were found (ps>.195).

Following this, there was the Acquisition stage (see Fig 1), during which participants viewed six presentations of the images from each set paired with one of two secondary images: the social-support figure image provided by the participants, or an image of a stranger that was gender-, age-, and ethnicity-matched to the social-support figure. One of the CS/secondary-image pairings from each set was consistently presented with a co-terminating 200ms electric shock (CS+/secondary-image pairing: 100% reinforcement schedule), while the other CS/secondary-image pairing was never paired with shock (CS-/secondary-image pairing). After the Acquisition stage, participants had a five minute long break during which they viewed a video clip about airplanes. Finally, during the Extinction stage, there were six non-reinforced presentations of each original neutral image once again presented alone, with the secondary image removed.

Fig 1. Acquisition and extinction procedures.

Example of the CS/secondary-image and shock pairings presented during the acquisition stage of the experiment and CS alone presentations during the extinction stage of the experiment. During acquisition, participants viewed two sets of two neutral images (clocks, stools), and both images from each set were paired with the same secondary image (social support figure, stranger). One of these pairings from each set was paired with shock, the CS+/secondary-image pairing, and one pairing was never paired with shock, the CS-/secondary-image pairing. Following acquisition was an extinction stage, during which each neutral image was once again presented on its own (no secondary image and no shock). Conditional fear acquisition was measured by comparing SCR for the CS+/secondary-image pairing to the CS-/secondary-image pairing within each set of neutral images during the acquisition stage. The numbers in parentheses indicate number of CS/secondary-image or CS alone presentations. All presentations were 6s followed by a 10s ISI. The order for both stages was pseudo-randomized, and counterbalanced across participants.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0175891.g001

Data analysis strategy

In order to examine fear learning patterns across conditions (social-support paired or stranger-paired), 2x2 repeated measures ANOVAs were run (paired image condition x reinforcement type) to examine mean SCR for the two CS+s and the two CS-s both during and post acquisition. If there was a significant interaction of paired image condition and reinforcement, it was considered that conditional fear was acquired differently for two conditions, and follow-up paired-sample t-tests were run to examine these differences.

In order to examine fear acquisition within each condition, paired-samples t-tests were run comparing acquisition means for the CS+/secondary-image pairing to the CS-/secondary-image pairing in the social-support-paired and stranger-paired conditions. If the SCR aroused by the CS+/secondary-image pairing was significantly higher than that of the CS-/secondary-image pairing, it was considered that a conditional fear response was acquired. Paired-samples t-tests were also run on the SCR aroused by the neutral images during the first trial of the extinction stage—the first trial after the secondary image had been removed and each neutral image was presented alone once again.

Additionally, we ran paired-samples t-tests to evaluate the effect of condition on fear acquisition, comparing mean difference scores (CS+/secondary-image vs. CS-/secondary-image) within each condition. Similarly, we ran paired-samples t-tests to evaluate fear responses post-acquisition, comparing SCR difference scores (CS+ vs. CS- from each condition) from the first trial of extinction.

Results

In order to determine the effect of the presence of a social-support image during fear acquisition, we first examined the effect of paired image condition (social support or stranger) and reinforcement type (CS+ or CS-) on fear responding. We found there was a significant interaction of these factors during the acquisition stage, when the paired images are still on the screen, F(1,19) = 10.326, p = .005, ηp2 = .352, as well as during the first trial post the acquisition stage, when the paired images have been removed, F(1,19) = 5.195, p = .034, ηp2 = .215, indicating that there are differences in fear learning across conditions.

Next, we examined whether there was a difference in fear acquisition across paired image conditions. We evaluated fear acquisition for both the social-support-paired and the stranger-paired conditions, and found that while participants did acquire fear for CS+s paired with strangers, t(19) = 4.86,p < .001, 95% CI[0.09,0.22], they did not acquire fear for CS+s paired with social-support figures, t(19) = .626,p = .539, 95% CI[-0.03,0.06], (see Fig 2A). Further examination showed that the effect of condition on fear acquisition was significant, t(19) = -3.80,p = .001, 95% CI[-0.21,-0.06], such that fear acquisition in presence of a social-support figure image was significantly less than fear acquisition in the presence of a stranger image. Together, these results demonstrate that the presence of social-support stimuli inhibits fear acquisition for other cues, providing support for our hypotheses.

Fig 2. Conditional fear acquisition.

A). SCR from the Acquisition stage: conditional fear responses were evaluated by comparing the CS+/secondary-image to the CS-/secondary-image from each condition (social-support-paired, stranger-paired). A conditional fear response was acquired in the stranger-paired condition, but not in the social-support-paired condition. B). SCR from the first trial of the Extinction stage: conditional fear responses were evaluated by comparing the CS+ and CS- from each condition when once again presented alone (with the social support or stranger image removed). A marginal conditional fear response was still present for the CS+ that had been paired with a stranger image, but not for the CS+ that had been paired with a social-support-figure image. All error bars indicate standard error. Asterisks indicate a statistically significant difference score (** indicates p< or = .001, * indicates p < .05), “+” indicates a marginal difference score (p < .1), and “ns” indicates a non-significant difference.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0175891.g002

In addition, we found that even after the secondary images were removed, a marginal fear response was still present in the stranger-paired condition, t(19) = 1.84,p = .082, 95% CI[-0.01,0.21], but there was no fear response present in the social-support-paired condition, t(19) = -1.52,p = .144, 95% CI[-0.16,0.02] (see Fig 2B). While the fear response for the stranger condition during this stage is only marginal, likely due to the relatively weak fear conditioning manipulation used here, it is trending toward significant and indicates that the fear association for the CS+ in the stranger-paired condition lasted beyond the end of the fear acquisition stage and the removal of the stranger image. Moreover, examination across conditions revealed that the fear response was significantly less in the social-support-paired condition than in the stranger-paired condition, t(19) = -2.28,p < .05, 95% CI[-0.31,-0.01]. Future work must build on this exploratory study to more closely examine the lasting effects of the presence of social support figures during fear acquisition.

Discussion

Social support has long been linked to positive health outcomes. One explanation for these health benefits is that social support buffers individuals against life stress, and it has been demonstrated that the presence of social-support reminders reduce both psychological and physiological responses to threat. However, no research to date has examined the relationship between social support and fear learning for other cues. In the current research, we examined whether social support not only signals safety and inhibits the fear response, but also reduces fear associations formed for other neutral cues. Results showed that the presence of social-support reminders inhibits the formation of fear associations. Specifically, we found that when an image of a social-support figure was paired with a neutral cue during fear acquisition, participants did not form a fear association for that cue, although they did form this association for a neutral cue paired with a stranger’s image.

Additional results showed that when presented alone after fear acquisition was completed, a marginal fear response remained for the neutral cue that had been paired with a stranger’s image, but there was none for the neutral cue that had been paired with a social-support figure’s image. Although these findings were only trending toward significant, they indicate that the benefits of social support continue even after an aversive event is over or a stressor is removed. This is interesting given that social integration, (participation in/a sense of belonging to a social network) has been shown to promote positive health outcomes even in the absence of current stress [for review, see: 26]. The current findings may give insight into the process underlying this effect—individuals with stronger social ties form fewer fear associations, while those who lack social ties form more fear associations, resulting in increased fear responding and stress as they interact with the world.

One possible alternative explanation for these findings is that the presence of stranger images augmented fear acquisition, as opposed to the presence of social support figure images reducing fear acquisition. However there are at least three reasons why this possibility seems unlikely. First, previous work using similar methods has demonstrated no difference in the safety or threat signaling function of images of strangers compared to images of neutral objects [25], indicating that stranger images would not be expected to have any impact on fear learning processes beyond that of neutral stimuli. Second, finding that the presence of stranger images augmented fear acquisition would imply that the fear conditioning procedure used here was not strong enough to produce fear acquisition except in the presence of strangers. Yet, similar fear conditioning procedures have been used in other studies by this team [25] and others [27–29], in which expected patterns of fear learning were produced, indicating that fear learning should occur under the current procedures. Finally, to the extent that the stranger faces were interpreted as threatening (though unlikely because all stranger stimuli were smiling faces which have been shown to be perceived as warm and approachable [30] and to yield reward-related neural activity [31]), this should actually lead to reduced fear acquisition to a separate conditional fear stimulus. Specifically, although fear acquisition is enhanced to threatening stimuli, the presence of a threatening stimulus in the context of learning fear to another cue actually prevents fear acquisition from occurring, a phenomenon known as blocking [32]. This is the opposite pattern of what we observed here. Thus, the results described here likely do not reflect an augmentation of fear acquisition caused by the presence of stranger stimuli, but rather a reduction of fear acquisition caused by the presence of social support stimuli.

This reduction in fear learning may stem from the ability of social-support stimuli to naturally, without any specific training, signal safety. It is possible that other characteristics of close others, such as being familiar or rewarding, could explain these effects. This is unlikely, however, given previous findings showing that while fear can be acquired for familiar or rewarding stimuli, it cannot be acquired for social-support stimuli [25]; nonetheless, future research is required to definitively rule out this possibility. Future work is also required to identify the boundaries of social support as a buffer against fear learning, such as investigating whether this effect is found when the conditional stimuli used are fear-relevant (e.g., prepared fear stimuli), or whether this effect is found in participants who are more prone to developing fears (e.g., anxious individuals). Similarly, while the current work could not address the role of gender in these effects, due to the limitation that data collected came from a sample that was 75% female, follow-up studies should investigate whether reduction of fear acquisition occurs equally across males and females.

In addition to exploring the boundaries of the buffering effects demonstrated here, future work must isolate the mechanism underlying the safety signaling properties of social support. While it is possible that social-support stimuli simply act as a buffer against pain, reducing the experience of shock, or increase feelings of safety in the moment, decreasing fear expression, a more likely explanation is that social-support stimuli alters the way in which fears are acquired. Social-support stimuli are known to trigger the release of endogenous opioids [for review, see: 33], which play a fundamental role in the error-correction process underlying fear learning [32,34], and therefore may disrupt the error-correction calculations that lead to fear acquisition. Further clarification of these mechanisms and effects will help develop a better understanding of how and when social support interferes with fear learning, bolstering positive health outcomes.

Altogether, these findings build on previous research demonstrating the buffering effects of social support and reveal a clearer picture of how social support might reduce psychological and physiological stress. By inhibiting the formation of fear associations for other cues, our close relationships may allow us to navigate the world with fewer learned fears, thus decreasing the activation of the threat response. Together with previous findings showing that social-support figures fulfill the requirements of prepared safety stimuli [25], these results suggest that social support may be helpful in preventing the formation of unnecessary or maladaptive fear associations and reducing threat related stress.

Supporting information

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Michael Fanselow, Ph.D. for his advice on fear-conditioning methods and theory.

Author Contributions

  1. Conceptualization: EAH NIE.
  2. Data curation: EAH.
  3. Formal analysis: EAH.
  4. Funding acquisition: EAH NIE.
  5. Investigation: EAH.
  6. Methodology: EAH NIE.
  7. Project administration: EAH.
  8. Resources: NIE.
  9. Software: EAH.
  10. Supervision: EAH NIE.
  11. Visualization: EAH.
  12. Writing – original draft: EAH NIE.
  13. Writing – review & editing: EAH NIE.

References

  1. 1. Cohen S, Doyle WJ, Skoner DP, Rabin BS, Gwaltney JM. Social ties and susceptibility to the common cold. Journal of the American Medical Association 1997; 277: 1940–1944. pmid:9200634
    • View Article
    • PubMed/NCBI
    • Google Scholar
  2. 2. Cohen S, Doyle WJ, Turner RB, Alper CM, Skoner DP. Sociability and susceptibility to the common cold. Psychological Science 2003; 14: 389–395. pmid:12930466
    • View Article
    • PubMed/NCBI
    • Google Scholar
  3. 3. House JS, Landis KR, Umberson D. Social relationships and health. Science 1988; 241: 540–545. pmid:3399889
    • View Article
    • PubMed/NCBI
    • Google Scholar
  4. 4. Hall JC. Some conditions of anxiety extinction. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 1955; 51: 126–132.
    • View Article
    • Google Scholar
  5. 5. Baum M. Extinction of avoidance response motivated by intense fear: social facilitation of the action of response prevention (flooding) in rats. Behaviour Research and Therapy 1969; 7: 57–62. pmid:5767625
    • View Article
    • PubMed/NCBI
    • Google Scholar
  6. 6. Davitz JR, Mason DJ. Socially facilitated reduction of a fear response in rats. Journal of comparative and physiological psychology (1955); 48: 149–151. pmid:13242680
    • View Article
    • PubMed/NCBI
    • Google Scholar
  7. 7. Liddell HS. Some specific factors that modify tolerance for environmental stress. In: Wolff HG, Wolff SG Jr., Hare CC, editors. Life stress and bodily disease, Baltimore, MD: Williams and Wilkins; 1950. pp. 155–171.
    • 8. Liddell HS. Conditioning and emotions. Scientific American 1954; 190: 48–57.
      • View Article
      • Google Scholar
    • 9. Ruis MAW, te Brake JHA, Buwalda B, De Boer SF, Meerlo P, Korte SM, et al. Housing familiar male wildtype rats together reduces the long-term adverse behavioural and physiological effects of social defeat. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1998; 24: 285–300.
      • View Article
      • Google Scholar
    • 10. Nakayasu T, Ishii K. Effects of pair-housing after social defeat experience on elevated plus-maze behavior in rats. Behavioural Processes 2008; 78: 477–480. pmid:18358638
      • View Article
      • PubMed/NCBI
      • Google Scholar
    • 11. Nakayasu T, Kato K. Is full physical contact necessary for buffering effects of pair housing on social stress in rats? Behavioural Processes 2011; 86: 230–235. pmid:21146595
      • View Article
      • PubMed/NCBI
      • Google Scholar
    • 12. Sachser S, Durschlag M, Hirzel D. Social relationships and the management of stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1998; 23: 891–904. pmid:9924743
      • View Article
      • PubMed/NCBI
      • Google Scholar
    • 13. Hennessy M, Zate R, Maken DS. Social buffering of the cortisol response of adult female guinea pigs. Psychology and Behavior 2008; 93: 883–888.
      • View Article
      • Google Scholar
    • 14. Cohen S, Hoberman HM. Positive events and social supports as buffers of life change stress. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 1983; 13: 99–125.
      • View Article
      • Google Scholar
    • 15. Cohen S, McKay G. Social support, stress, and the buffering hypothesis: a theoretical analysis. In: Baum A, Taylor SE, Singer JE, editors. Handbook of psychology and health. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1984. pp. 253–267.
      • 16. Kessler RC, McLeod JD. Social support and mental health in community samples. In: Cohen S, Syme LS, editors. Social support and health. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 1985. pp. 219–240.
        • 17. Eisenberger NI, Taylor SE, Gable SL, Hilmert CJ, Lieberman MD. Neural pathways link social support to attenuated neuroendocrine stress responses. NeuroImage 2007; 35: 1601–1612. pmid:17395493
          • View Article
          • PubMed/NCBI
          • Google Scholar
        • 18. Gerin W, Pieper C, Levy R, Pickering TG. Social support in social interaction: a moderator of cardiovascular reactivity. Psychosomatic Medicine 1992; 54: 324–336. pmid:1620808
          • View Article
          • PubMed/NCBI
          • Google Scholar
        • 19. Thorsteinsson EB, James JE, Gregg ME. Effects of video-relayed social support on hemodynamic reactivity and salivary cortisol during laboratory-based behavioral challenge. Health Psychology 1998; 17: 436–444. pmid:9776002
          • View Article
          • PubMed/NCBI
          • Google Scholar
        • 20. Thorsteinsson EB, James JE. A meta-analysis of the effects of experimental manipulations of social support during laboratory stress. Psychology and Health 1999; 14: 869–886.
          • View Article
          • Google Scholar
        • 21. Brown JL, Sheffield D, Leary MR, Robinson ME. Social support and experimental pain. Psychosomatic Medicine 2003; 65: 276–283. pmid:12651995
          • View Article
          • PubMed/NCBI
          • Google Scholar
        • 22. Master SL, Eisenberger NI, Taylor SE, Naliboff BD, Shirinyan D, Lieberman MD. A picture’s worth: partner photographs reduce experimentally induced pain. Psychological Science 2009; 20: 1316–1318. pmid:19788531
          • View Article
          • PubMed/NCBI
          • Google Scholar
        • 23. Younger J, Aron A, Parke S, Chatterjee N, Mackey S. Viewing pictures of a romantic partner reduces experimental pain: involvement of neural reward systems. PLoS ONE 2010; 5: e13309. pmid:20967200
          • View Article
          • PubMed/NCBI
          • Google Scholar
        • 24. Eisenberger NI, Master SL, Inagaki TI, Taylor SE, Shirinyan D, Lieberman MD, et al. Attachment figures activate a safety signal-related neural region and reduce pain experience. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science 2011; 108: 11721–11726.
          • View Article
          • Google Scholar
        • 25. Hornstein EA, Fanselow MS, Eisenberger NI. A safe haven: Social-support figures are prepared safety stimuli. Psychological Science 2016; 27: 1051–1060. pmid:27324266
          • View Article
          • PubMed/NCBI
          • Google Scholar
        • 26. Cohen S, Social relationships and health. American Psychologist 2004; 59: 676–684. pmid:15554821
          • View Article
          • PubMed/NCBI
          • Google Scholar
        • 27. Schiller D., Cain C.K., Curley N.G., Schwartz J.S. Stern S.A., LeDoux J.E., & Phelps E.A. Evidence for recovery of fear following immediate extinction in rats and humans. Learning & Memory, 2008; 15: 394–402.
          • View Article
          • Google Scholar
        • 28. Olsson A. & Phelps E.A. Learned fear of “unseen” faces after Pavlovian, observational, and instructed fear. Psychological Science, 2004; 15: 822–828. pmid:15563327
          • View Article
          • PubMed/NCBI
          • Google Scholar
        • 29. Olsson A, Ebert JP, Banaji MR, Phelps EA. The role of social groups in the persistence of learned fear. Science 2005; 309: 785–787. pmid:16051800
          • View Article
          • PubMed/NCBI
          • Google Scholar
        • 30. Lau S. The effect of smiling on person perception. Journal of Social Psychology, 1982; 117: 63–67.
          • View Article
          • Google Scholar
        • 31. Rademacher L, Krach S, Kohls G, Irmak A, Grunder G, & Spreckelmeyer KN. Dissociation of neural networks for anticipation and consumption of monetary and social rewards. NeuroImage, 2010; 49: 3276–3285. pmid:19913621
          • View Article
          • PubMed/NCBI
          • Google Scholar
        • 32. Fanselow MS. Pavlovian conditioning, negative feedback, and blocking: mechanisms that regulate association formation. Neuron 1998; 20: 625–627. pmid:9581755
          • View Article
          • PubMed/NCBI
          • Google Scholar
        • 33. Nelson E, Panksepp J. Brain substrates of infant-mother attachment: Contributions of Opioids, Oxytocin, and Norepinephrine. Neuroscience and Behavioral Reviews 1998; 22: 437–452.
          • View Article
          • Google Scholar
        • 34. Fanselow MS. Naloxone and pavlovian fear conditioning. Learning and Motivation 1981; 12: 398–419.
          • View Article
          • Google Scholar

        Which of the following is most strongly suggested as the main neurotransmitter promoting sleep?

        GABAergic Neurons GABA has been traditionally viewed as a sleep-promoting neurotransmitter. Most sleep-promoting populations are GABAergic, including those in the preoptic area (Sherin et al, 1996) and parafacial zone (Anaclet et al, 2014).

        Which of the following is one of the leading causes of disability and death psychology?

        Depression is one of the leading causes of disability. Suicide is the fourth leading cause of death among 15-29-year-olds. People with severe mental health conditions die prematurely – as much as two decades early – due to preventable physical conditions.

        Which of the following statements is true of the preliminary hypotheses about the interaction that causes vulnerability to disease?

        Which of the following statements is true of the preliminary hypotheses about the interaction that causes vulnerability to disease? Stressful situations make individuals more susceptible to diseases.